'Happy ending effect' can bias future decisions
Listen to this article
A new study has unearthed three fascinating facts about the way we go about making decisions based on past experience:
- Our brains can’t always reliably evaluate experiences that unfold over time
- We tend to give disproportionate weight to the later part of an experience
- This can lead to bad decisions when choosing whether to repeat an experience
Humans are hard-wired to prefer experiences that end well, and the influence of previous experience declines the longer ago it happened. This means we can’t always trust that choices we make based on previous experience will serve our best interests in the future.
New research published in The Journal of Neuroscience, has revealed that two different parts of the brain are activated, and compete with each other, when we make decisions based on past experience. They can cause us to overvalue experiences that end well despite starting badly and undervalue experiences that end badly despite starting well—even if both are equally valuable overall.
What the researchers say: “When you’re deciding where to go for dinner, for example, you think about where you’ve had a good meal in the past. But your memory of whether that meal was good isn’t always reliable—our brain values the final few moments of the experience more highly than the rest of it,” said the lead researcher. “If we can’t control our in-built attraction to happy endings, then we can’t trust our choices to serve our best interests.”
The part of the brain called the amygdala works out the ‘objective value’ of an experience—the overall tastiness of a three-course meal, for example. Meanwhile a brain region called the anterior insula was shown to ‘mark down’ our valuation of an experience if it gets gradually worse over time.
The further back in time an experience was, even if still quite recent, the less weight it carries in making the next decision. The researchers call it the ‘happy ending effect’: we tend to make decisions based on previous experiences that ended well, irrespective of how good the experiences were overall.
In the study, twenty-seven healthy male volunteers were asked to choose which of two pots of coins, viewed on-screen one at a time, had the greatest total value. They watched as coins of varying sizes - representing their value—fell from the pots in quick succession, while a brain scanner revealed what was happening in their brain using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The task was repeated several times with different sequences of coins.
The volunteers systematically chose the wrong pot when the coins decreased in size towards the end of the sequence. This reveals that the brain was imposing a penalty on the whole sequence, irrespective of its total value, when the ending was not good. The effect varied from person to person, but only a few were able to ignore it entirely and make an accurate decision.
The results verify theoretical models of decision-making and challenge the popular belief that sub-optimal decision-making is routed in the amygdala—a primitive part of our brain shared with just about all creatures with a brain—whereas more astute reasoning happens in the more evolved part. They show that our evaluation of an extended experience is encoded robustly in the amygdala.
The attraction to the final moments of an experience is a fundamental mechanism in the human brain and important to be aware of, say the researchers. While there are clear advantages to paying attention to whether things are on an upward or downward trajectory, our judgements can fail us when we try to evaluate an overall experience afterwards.
While bad decision-making in the context of eating out might not be disastrous, this inaccurate valuation in summarizing past events could lead to bad choices when using the information to make decisions for the longer-term—for example, deciding which politician to vote for.
“Our attraction to the quality of the final moment of an experience is exploited by politicians seeking re-election; they will always try to appear strong and successful towards the end of their time in office,” the lead author said. “If you fall for this trick, and disregard historical incompetence and failure, then you might end up re-electing an unfit politician.” You mean like DT?
So, what? This research underlines the fundamental irrationality of human (as opposed to chimp) decision-making.
As the researchers note, we assume that if something ends well that that upward trajectory will continue. One of the reasons for this is that our brains are essentially forward-looking (unless we suffer from depression) and we tend to disregard the past.
Thus, we will trust someone to fulfil a promise if they make out a case that they are capable of fulfilling it, even if they have let us down multiple times in the past. The battered wife syndrome is a perfect example of this: “But he will change. He seems so sorry for what he’s done and promises not to do it again.” The promise is the upward trajectory which the brain latches onto, the reality of the past is lost.
Join the discussion
More from this issue of TR
You might be interested in
Back to Today's ResearchJoin our tribe
Subscribe to Dr. Bob Murray’s Today’s Research, a free weekly roundup of the latest research in a wide range of scientific disciplines. Explore leadership, strategy, culture, business and social trends, and executive health.